Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Altering neuronal excitability to preserve network connectivity in a computational model of Alzheimer's disease

  • Willem de Haan ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    w.dehaan@vumc.nl

    Affiliations Department of Clinical Neurophysiology and MEG, VUmc, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Alzheimer Center and Department of Neurology, VUmc, Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

  • Elisabeth C. W. van Straaten,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Clinical Neurophysiology and MEG, VUmc, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

  • Alida A. Gouw,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Clinical Neurophysiology and MEG, VUmc, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Alzheimer Center and Department of Neurology, VUmc, Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

  • Cornelis J. Stam

    Roles Methodology, Resources, Software, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Clinical Neurophysiology and MEG, VUmc, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Abstract

Neuronal hyperactivity and hyperexcitability of the cerebral cortex and hippocampal region is an increasingly observed phenomenon in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease (AD). In later stages, oscillatory slowing and loss of functional connectivity are ubiquitous. Recent evidence suggests that neuronal dynamics have a prominent role in AD pathophysiology, making it a potentially interesting therapeutic target. However, although neuronal activity can be manipulated by various (non-)pharmacological means, intervening in a highly integrated system that depends on complex dynamics can produce counterintuitive and adverse effects. Computational dynamic network modeling may serve as a virtual test ground for developing effective interventions. To explore this approach, a previously introduced large-scale neural mass network with human brain topology was used to simulate the temporal evolution of AD-like, activity-dependent network degeneration. In addition, six defense strategies that either enhanced or diminished neuronal excitability were tested against the degeneration process, targeting excitatory and inhibitory neurons combined or separately. Outcome measures described oscillatory, connectivity and topological features of the damaged networks. Over time, the various interventions produced diverse large-scale network effects. Contrary to our hypothesis, the most successful strategy was a selective stimulation of all excitatory neurons in the network; it substantially prolonged the preservation of network integrity. The results of this study imply that functional network damage due to pathological neuronal activity can be opposed by targeted adjustment of neuronal excitability levels. The present approach may help to explore therapeutic effects aimed at preserving or restoring neuronal network integrity and contribute to better-informed intervention choices in future clinical trials in AD.

Author summary

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a growing burden on society, without a cure in sight. Pathological high neuronal activity and excitability is an increasingly observed phenomenon in early stage AD. Its exact role in the disease process is unclear, but it may form an interesting therapeutic target. However, although brain dynamics can be influenced in many ways, the highly complex nature of the brain makes it difficult to predict what approach will be most effective. To test our hypothesis that neuronal hyperactivity can be countered effectively by altering neuronal excitability levels, we examined various strategies aimed at preserving brain network integrity in a computational AD model of the human brain. Of these strategies, a scenario involving stimulation of excitatory neurons extends the period with normal network function most successfully. The results of this ‘virtual trial’ suggest that network effects of pathological neuronal activity can be opposed by selective altering of neuronal excitability levels. In general, this approach can explore therapeutic effects aimed at preserving or restoring brain network integrity, and thereby contribute to selecting promising interventions for future clinical trials in AD.

Introduction

Electrophysiologically, Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is commonly characterized by ‘negative’ findings: a gradual, diffuse slowing of brain activity (notably the posterior dominant rhythm), decreases in functional connectivity, and a loss of network structure and complexity [13]. In recent years however, various studies have reported observations that challenge this notion: in the preclinical AD, Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) and early AD stages, neuronal hyperactivity and increased functional connectivity has been observed at various scales [411]. While these increases were first interpreted as a compensation mechanism for synaptic dysfunction in AD, the evidence now clearly points in a different direction; neuronal hyperactivity as a part of the pathophysiological cascade in AD. For instance, it has been shown that damage induced by the typical amyloid deposits in AD leads to neuronal hyperexcitability and disinhibition [12,13]. In turn, neuronal hyperactivity itself has also been demonstrated to drive amyloid deposition rates [14,15], a very intriguing finding that has led some to hypothesize that neuronal dynamics may play a causal role in AD pathophysiology, possibly as part of a positive feedback loop involving the neurotoxic amyloid deposits [1618]. Additional, indirect support for this view is provided by the very early phase in which neuronal dynamics and connectivity are disrupted in AD, the independent associations of AD risk factors (age and ApoE4 status) with neuronal activity levels, and the increased incidence of epilepsy and epileptiform neuronal activity in this population [6,9,1927].

Regardless of the exact role of disrupted neuronal dynamics in the pathological cascade of AD, the observations described above may have therapeutic relevance, since neuronal or synaptic function can be targeted effectively by various (non-) pharmacological means. In fact, the current medicinal (symptomatic) treatment for AD aims to improve neuronal communication by enhancing cholinergic or glutamatergic neurotransmission [2830]. Unfortunately, the large majority of the current clinical trials are aimed at decreasing amyloid load, while investigations on improving neuronal function are relatively limited. Nevertheless, a few recent studies have confirmed that targeting neuronal or synaptic behavior may be beneficial in AD. For example, experimental studies in rats and in humans with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) indicated that the anti-epileptic drug levetiracetam diminishes hippocampal hyperactivity while improving cognitive performance [31]. Also, enhancement of synapse formation and function by medical nutrition was reported to have a positive effect on memory function in AD patients [32,33]. Non-pharmacological therapeutic studies with the specific aim to modify neuronal activity (e.g. deep brain stimulation or transcranial magnetic stimulation) have not yet produced significant clinical benefits in AD, but are under development [3436].

With these possibilities in mind, a key question is how to predict and optimize the effect of therapy in the complex, highly interconnected and highly dynamic human brain. While therapeutic strategies usually target specific brain areas or structures, it is naïve to expect that local interference will not influence surrounding or connected parts of the brain. As our awareness of the brain as a complex, distributed system is growing, we should appreciate the consequences: even subtle changes can have wide-ranging and paradoxical effects [37,38]. Variations in efficacy and unforeseen or adverse effects are frequently encountered in neurological therapy, and perhaps could be partly explained by our lack of insight in this regard. How can we get a grip on this complex behavior, in order to guide our hypotheses and experiments more reliably?

In recent years, studies of the complexity of the brain have entered a new era, due to rapid advances in data acquisition technology and the powerful application of theoretical concepts of complex network analysis [39,40]. In clinically oriented studies, this method has mainly been employed to interpret patient data in order to gain a better understanding of disease mechanisms or to find diagnostic and prognostic markers of disease [41,42]. However, top-down patient-driven research is not its sole application: the combination of network analysis and computational modeling can offer an interesting complementary bottom-up approach [4345]. For example, several studies have combined neural mass modeling with network analysis to study the effect of lesions on the brain [38,46]. With regard to AD, a previous study from our group indicated that neuronal hyperactivity may play a substantial role in the disease mechanism by demonstrating that an activity-dependent degeneration regime yielded remarkably similar results to studies in AD and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) patients, including selective hub vulnerability and initial neuronal hyperactivity followed by slowing, disconnection, and loss of network topology [18,47].

Despite the theoretical advances, complex network analysis of the brain has not yet led to any improvement for AD patients. Although abstract by nature, in our opinion network modeling might facilitate a transition towards clinical applications: in addition to simulating degenerative or lesion effects, models may also be employed to explore therapeutic intervention strategies, in order to make dynamic network changes more predictable and easier to understand in terms of their underlying mechanisms. Therefore, we used our computational AD degeneration model for the present study, but now with the addition of various ‘therapeutic’ network interventions. In the model, neuronal excitability levels of both excitatory and inhibitory neurons can be adjusted selectively (roughly simulating medication effects or nonpharmacological stimulation/inhibition techniques). Since neuronal hyperactivity was the main driver of network degeneration in this model, we hypothesized that the strategy employing global neuronal inhibition would be most effective in countering this process. Fig 1 provides an overview of the overall workflow.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Global overview of relevant modeling and analysis procedures.

This study focuses on the parts indicated in red: the virtual trial. The general workflow of our analysis can be described as follows: the dynamic network model is run with the degeneration algorithm and, simultaneously, one of the interventions (or no intervention). Hence, the network is damaged over time according to local neuronal activity levels, but at the same time, by changing neuronal excitability levels due to varied threshold potential (Vd) settings (see below for details), a counterstrategy is employed to diminish the effect of the damage and maintain network topology close to the original state. The resulting oscillatory, connectivity and network topology changes are then described using the selected measures (see below) to evaluate the effect of the different interventions over time, and finally these are compared statistically to obtain an impression of the most successful strategy. For a more detailed stepwise description of the analysis, please refer to the Method section.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005707.g001

Results

In each experiment, six intervention strategies were compared to the ‘no intervention’ (degeneration on, no intervention) and ‘control’ (no degeneration, no intervention) conditions: global stimulation, global inhibition, selective stimulation or inhibition of all excitatory neurons, and selective excitation or inhibition of all inhibitory neurons (see Fig 2). Different levels of stimulation and inhibition were tested (see method section). Within the individual strategies, adjusting the excitability levels produced fairly similar results (i.e. different threshold potential (Vd, see method section and S2 Text) and for clarity purposes we chose a single representative value for each strategy to be depicted in the figures below.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Influence of intervention strategies on the neural mass model.

All black lines are connections that are damaged by the ADD mechanism. Note that due to the interplay between excitatory and inhibitory forces and the network topology, the effect of an intervention on a network level is not easy to foresee. All strategies were continuous. Exc. = excitatory neurons, Inh. = inhibitory neurons.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005707.g002

To assess functional network status during the degeneration period, several relevant quantitative markers were employed, highlighting three main aspects of functional network quality: spectral power, functional connectivity and network topology (see Table 1). In addition, the effect of degeneration on structural connectivity was compared between the most successful strategy and the ‘no intervention’ mode, using the normalized node strength (see method section). Using this strategy, the effect of intervention timing was also explored. Note that all intervention strategies were commenced after ten degeneration cycles (T = 10), to simulate a disease condition for which therapy was started with a delay (see also figure 6 of [18]), except in the intervention timing tests (experiment 6). Graphs depict only the intervention period.

thumbnail
Table 1. Overview of network analysis measures.

Measure selection was based on the aim to describe the most robust and relevant functional network changes in AD. For exact definitions please refer to the method section and Supporting Information S4.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005707.t001

Experiment 1a: Spectral analysis–relative alpha power

Relative power in the lower alpha band (8–10 Hz) inevitably decreased during the degeneration process, but the rate at which this happens differed substantially between the various strategies (see Fig 3A). Without intervention (black line), lower alpha power breaks down around T = 20, decreasing to a level between 0.1 and 0.2. Global inhibition, inhibition of inhibitory neurons, and especially stimulation of excitatory neurons was able to prolong a normal level of relative power in the lower alpha band (around 0.8) and postpone the subsequent collapse.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Spectral analysis.

A) Comparison of relative lower alpha (8–10 Hz) power values over time for the various interventions. B) Comparison of peak frequency level over time for the various interventions. Error bars indicate the standard deviation over the total number of runs of the various strategies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005707.g003

Experiment 1b: Spectral analysis–peak frequency

The gradual decrease of the (posterior) alpha peak value is a robust finding in AD, and levels lower than 8 Hz are generally regarded as pathologic [48]. Without intervention, the alpha peak is stable for a short while, but starts to decrease steadily around T = 25. The same pattern was observed during all intervention strategies, although the peak level stayed above 8 Hz for a substantially longer period. This seems to be mainly due to a higher initial peak value (Fig 3B).

Experiment 2: Functional connectivity–Phase Lag Index (PLI)

Overall functional connectivity in the network showed a critical transition period around T = 20 with a breakdown even more abrupt than in the lower alpha power results (see Fig 4). In the ‘no intervention’ condition, PLI values were much higher than in the control network at the start of therapy due to the ADD process, but it rapidly fell to low levels. The ‘stimulation of excitatory neurons’ regime was able to maintain normal/high PLI values most successfully, but suddenly dropped down to the same level as the other scenarios around T = 30.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Functional connectivity analysis: comparison of Phase lag index (PLI) values over time for the different interventions.

Error bars indicate the standard deviation over the total number of runs of the various strategies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005707.g004

Experiment 3a: Network topology–Local connectivity

AD network degradation as assessed by EEG/MEG has previously been characterized by a decrease of the (normalized) clustering coefficient [49,50], contributing to the loss of presumably efficient small-world network topology. Here, a pattern similar to the PLI results developed, and with the global inhibition, inhibition of inhibitory neurons and stimulation of excitatory neurons as most beneficial regimes (Fig 5A).

thumbnail
Fig 5. Network topology.

A) Network analysis: comparison of normalized clustering coefficient (gamma) values over time for the different interventions, indicating local connectivity strength. B) Comparison of modularity (community presence) over time for the different interventions. C) Comparison of algebraic connectivity values over time for the different interventions, indicating network robustness. D) Comparison of MST leaf number values over time for the different interventions, reflecting network hub presence. Error bars indicate the standard deviation over the total number of runs of the various strategies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005707.g005

Experiment 3b: Network topology–Modularity

Macroscopic brain connectivity networks have been shown to possess a (hierarchical) modular structure, a feature that is presumed to promote network efficiency and robustness. In AD, modularity gradually weakens [51]. ‘Stimulation of excitatory neurons’ seemed to retain normal modular structure relatively well (Fig 5B).

Experiment 3c: Network topology–Robustness

Algebraic Connectivity is a graph spectral measure of overall network robustness, higher values signifying a network that is harder to tear apart [52,53]. Strategies that were relatively successful in upholding the robustness of the network were ‘global inhibition’, ‘inhibition of inhibitory neurons’ and ‘stimulation of excitatory neurons’ (Fig 5C).

Experiment 3d: Network topology–Hub presence

It has been reported previously that network hub structure is lost in AD [54]. In this simulation similar findings became apparent, with a later decrease of the MST Leaf number in the most successful scenarios, ‘inhibition of excitatory neurons’, ‘global inhibition’ and ‘stimulation of excitatory neurons’.

Experiment 4: Structural network connectivity–Normalized Node Strength

Structural connectivity decreases inevitably over time in the model, but this process can be delayed by interventions. Fig 6 compares the ‘no intervention’ condition (blue lines) with the relatively successful stimulation of excitatory neurons (red lines). For different time points normalized node strength is plotted against original structural degree. This measure is the ratio of present structural degree over its original degree, so a value smaller than 1 indicates a loss of structural connectivity. The loss was not equal for all nodes; hubs tend to decrease more, confirming the previously reported hub vulnerability (18,49,55). The result of the intervention was that initially damage is counteracted better (see T = 20), but ultimately the network apparently suffered more than in the control state (see T = 40). In the intervention condition hub vulnerability is less pronounced; the slope of the lines is less steep. Remarkably, at T = 30, overall structural connectivity was fairly equal between the conditions, while functional measures differed considerably (see results above).

thumbnail
Fig 6. Effect of the ‘stimulation of excitatory neurons’ intervention on structural connectivity.

At three different time points, structural connectivity strength is compared with the ‘no intervention’ condition (blue lines). This is done in six different categories, based on structural connectivity level (degree). Highly connected hubs fall in the sixth category, most right in the chart. Note: error bars indicate standard deviations based on all node degrees per category. For a detailed list of region names and degree distributions see [18, Table 1].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005707.g006

Experiment 5: Intervention timing

To assess the effect of the specific delay with which the intervention is started, we compared functional connectivity results (PLI) for three different intervention starting points (T = 0, T = 10 and T = 20), using the relatively successful ‘stimulation of excitatory neurons’ strategy. As can be judged from Fig 7, the response of the PLI to the differently timed interventions was similar, and their ability to uphold PLI values near or above the control network levels seems mainly dependent on the moment in the degeneration process at which the intervention was initiated.

thumbnail
Fig 7. Intervention timing.

Overall functional connectivity (Phase Lag Index, PLI) over time is compared between the ‘no intervention’ condition, a healthy control network, and the most successful intervention, ‘stimulation of excitatory neurons’, started at three different points in time. Note that the graph starts at T = 10; the ‘early’ intervention has been active ten cycles before this graph starts. Error bars indicate the standard deviation over the total number of runs of the various strategies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005707.g007

Overall intervention performance

Overall performance of the four most successful strategies was assessed by combining results into a total sum per category for the four most successful interventions (Fig 8, see caption or method section for score definition). This enables a comparison at a glance between the various strategies regarding different qualities, and show that all these strategies bring an overall improvement compared to the ‘no intervention’ state, with individual differences. The most successful strategy was the ‘stimulation of excitatory neurons’ intervention, but also inhibitory strategies performed fairly well.

thumbnail
Fig 8. Overall intervention performance.

Performance score of the 4 most successful strategies compared to the ‘no intervention’ condition. Bar height is based on a total score per category, derived by summing the amount of time that measures within that category stay near a normal level, which is then divided by the ‘no intervention’ score of that category to derive a performance ratio.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005707.g008

Discussion

In this study we used a simplified neurophysiological human brain network model to investigate the effects of altering neuronal excitability during an activity-dependent degeneration scenario that produces AD-like functional network deterioration. We conclude that, in this computational model, the most successful strategy to preserve network integrity is, contrary to our hypothesis, a selective stimulation of excitatory neurons. A virtual network intervention approach as described here may help to predict the impact of interventions on the human brain.

Explaining the success of neuronal stimulation in a hyperactive network

The observation that the ‘stimulation of excitatory neurons’ strategy is most successful in retaining network organization in a situation where neuronal activity leads to damage seems contradictory at least, and is in stark contrast with our initial hypothesis. Intuitively, one would be inclined to choose a strategy to slow down the degeneration process by either inhibiting excitation or by stimulating inhibition, and, judging from our results, the inhibitory strategies perform quite well. Several pharmacological studies aimed at countering aberrant neuronal hyperactivity provide circumstantial support for this view [31,55]. However, as can be judged from the present experiments, the effects of the different types of stimulation and inhibition are quite unpredictable: in this particular topology, the increased excitability in excitatory neurons apparently leads to a condition where desirable network topology is retained longer. Although this result begs for an mechanistic explanation, this was not the primary goal of our current experiment. However, we did perform further analyses to better understand the outcome (see also S3 Text): we hypothesized that one reason for a beneficial effect might be that the net effect of stimulation of excitatory neurons on the entire neural mass is inhibitory, due to the interplay with the relatively influential inhibitory neuron groups. However, analysis showed that this is not the case. Since all neural masses are equal except for their connectivity pattern, the explanation may lie in the network topology itself. For example, the successful scenarios seem to suppress functional hub strength of the network (see Fig 5D), and while hubs may generally improve network efficiency, they can also facilitate the spread of pathology[56]. Different topologies should be tested with the same degeneration algorithm in future experiments to explore this idea. An alternative explanation, is that the philosophy behind a strategy should perhaps not be based on trying to oppose the degeneration mechanism, but on keeping the network ‘alive’ for as long as possible in general by stimulating the remaining neurons in a decaying network; compensatory activation. However, inhibitory strategies performed quite well in our experiments, and current literature clearly points to the pathological aspect of the hyperactivity in AD [5759].

Strategy selection and timing

With the present degeneration algorithm, intervention success is relative, since regardless of the chosen strategy, network breakdown was inevitable. This may be explained by specific features of the algorithm, as described in the study limitations paragraph. Another issue is our selection of strategies. As a first exploration we opted for global, continuous interventions, but many more scenarios are possible, and might lead to different outcomes: selective stimulation or inhibition of specific regions (for example hub regions), different delay or timing of interventions, interventions based on individual connectivity patterns, adaptive interventions, structural interventions, and so on. A present-day major objective in the field of neurodegenerative disease research is the aim for early intervention: there is a growing consensus that, to be successful, therapy should be commenced before structural pathology is widespread [60]. We examined the influence of three different intervention starting points (see Fig 7) on functional connectivity (PLI), an outcome measure that is the basis of all graph theoretical analysis, and a measure that produced clear differences among the various strategies. Here however, no clear benefit of early intervention initiation is found; in fact, the response of the network to the ‘stimulation of excitatory neurons’ intervention seems independent of the moment in de degeneration process. This is surprising, since underlying structural connectivity and topology is inevitably weaker at later time points, raising the question whether some kind of adaptive therapy could be effective, i.e. starting stimulation at low PLI levels, and stopping at high levels.

Another intriguing finding was the sharp peak in activity and connectivity just prior to sudden decrease in value in several of the scenarios, including the most successful ones, reminiscent of a non-linear system near a critical transition (close to breakdown in this case). Although we did not elaborate on this, this finding may also point to the potential merit of an adaptive strategy that can avoid the increases and thereby prevent network collapse.

The definition of a successful strategy

Also, the success of a therapy depends on how ‘success’ is defined. In this exploratory study, we adhered to the general idea of keeping a system in or close to its original functional state over time, which seems straightforward and in line with goals in medical practice in general. Of course, in our model, the virtual time base does not permit statements about actual timelines or tempo of deterioration, but when a strategy is able to uphold certain desired characteristics approximately twice as long (see for example Fig 4) as in the ‘no-intervention’ state, this can reasonably be thought of as successful; real-life therapy that delays degenerative damage effects in AD this long would be considered a spectacular improvement. Although a translation of our present findings to clinical experiments and pharmacological recommendations is too ambitious at this point, an appealing thought is that the current medication for AD leads to stimulation of excitatory neurons, the most successful strategy in our experiments as well. Ultimately, intervention modeling studies will require validation through real interventions targeting brain activity, to evaluate their true value.

Study limitations

Various arbitrary choices that were made in this exploratory study may have had an influence on its outcome. For example, one might wonder whether the model is sufficiently detailed to realistically simulate cerebral dynamics. Neural masses were identical (except for their connectivity characteristics), while there are distinct regional differences in the brain. Also, the number of masses (n = 78) could be augmented based on more recent and detailed structural connectome datasets. Our present DTI-based structural connectivity data in the model is based on deterministic tractography, which has been demonstrated to be inadequate for detecting crossing white matter fibers, and newer, more powerful methods are available [61,62]. Repeating the current approach using alternative connectome data is an important way to validate model findings, and should be performed in future studies. Another interesting recent approach is to replace the structural network by one that is based on a generative model itself [6365]. Also, one could argue that connectome data taken from an older control group would have led to different, more representative results. Furthermore, the neural masses themselves model a neuronal region macroscopically, but not on a microscopic level, while detailed neurophysiological models at (sub)cellular level are certainly available. Could more detail have led to a different outcome? While that certainly is possible, we have several motives for our current approach. First, the global macroscopic level of analysis is of particular interest, since clinical measurements (e.g. EEG, (f)MRI) are at this level; detailed in vivo measurements on a microscopical level in memory clinic patients are not (yet) feasible. Second, the observation that, even in this relatively basic model of human connectivity, the outcomes of fairly straightforward interventions defy an easy mechanistic explanation underscore the complexity of network effects, and the need for modeling. Third, data analysis time and computing power are finite, and added detail can increase demands placed on these aspects exponentially.

Other limitations stem from specific choices that are made in the activity-dependent degeneration (ADD) algorithm that was used in this study. The algorithm was chosen because it is based on the singular assumption that neuronal hyperactivity leads to dysfunction and damage, and because of its strong resemblance with Alzheimer-related network degeneration, including features like early-stage disinhibition and hub vulnerability [18]. However, in the ADD algorithm the network is damaged regardless of the absolute level of activity; even regions with a normal level of activity will be damaged, although exponentially less (see ‘loss function’ in S1 Text). A degeneration regime that spares regions that exhibit a normal range of neuronal activity might be more plausible (metabolically), and could result in strategies that will be able to maintain network integrity for a longer, perhaps even indefinite, period of time. Also, neuronal plasticity as a defense mechanism to degeneration was not incorporated in this model, but may yield more realistic results and enhance network survival. For example, mechanisms such as synchronization-dependent or growth-dependent plasticity could be implemented in de model [66]. Our rather ‘unforgiving’ degeneration mechanism may have underestimated the impact of the various therapies, and alternative algorithm choices may lead to different intervention outcomes. However, adding more assumptions (and complexity) to the model or its degeneration algorithm will probably enlarge the unpredictability of the interventions. Finally, the chosen Vd stepsize of 0.5 in our analysis was a pragmatic choice, and although our Vd analysis in the Supporting Information (S2 Text) suggests that Vd values in between would not produce very different results, a smaller stepsize may have been more insightful.

Clinical relevance and future directions

With the growing interest in the human connectome in general, and more specifically in connectivity as a biomarker of neurodegenerative disease [67,68], intervention modeling is a logical step forward. Computer simulations serving as a virtual test ground for intervening in complex systems is common practice in related fields, such as economy, meteorology or systems biology. With the advent of enhanced acquisition techniques such as DTI and MEG, enabling increasingly accurate human connectivity datasets, and of increasing computational power, analysis will become more elaborate, faster and hopefully more user-friendly and available to clinical researchers. Two long-term aims seem meaningful to pursue: the further development of dynamic connectome (“dynome”) intervention models, and the validation of these models with clinical experiments, perhaps involving individual structural connectome data to achieve personalized results. This way, the modeling approach may become a viable intermediate step in the bidirectional interpretation of both etiological hypotheses and clinical experiments in connectivity-related research in general [56].

Conclusion

The observations in this computational modeling study surprisingly suggest that AD-like network degeneration due to neuronal hyperactivity can be countered most effectively by global stimulation of excitatory neurons. The wide-ranging and unpredictable impact of intervention strategies in this straightforward dynamic human connectome model, with a limited number of identical neural masses and macro scale cerebral topology, illustrates the fact that interventions in complex systems can lead to counterintuitive results. In general, network intervention analysis may help to explore and explain therapeutic effects aimed at preserving network integrity, and thereby potentially guide the design and hypothesis selection of clinical intervention trials.

Methods

Study design

For this study we simulated neurophysiologic activity of cortical regions embedded in a realistic structural network topology to evaluate hypotheses about the relation between neuronal activity and (structural and functional) connectivity. The output of this model provides information about neuronal activity in the form of average voltage and spike density per region, and generates EEG-like data that can be subjected to further analysis. Furthermore, hypotheses about brain pathophysiology can be tested by artificially changing structural or dynamical properties of the model. The general workflow of our analysis can be described as follows (see also Fig 1 for a graphical overview): the dynamic network model is run with the degeneration algorithm and, simultaneously, one of the interventions (or no intervention). This means that the network is damaged according to local neuronal activity levels, but at the same time, by changing neuronal excitability levels due to varied threshold potential (Vd) settings (see below for details), a counterstrategy is employed to diminish the effect of the damage and maintain network topology close to the original state. The resulting oscillatory, connectivity and network topology changes are then described using the selected quantitative outcome measures (see below) to evaluate the effect of the different interventions over time, and in the final step these are compared statistically to obtain an impression of the most successful strategy.

Network dynamics: Description of the Neural Mass Model

We used a model of interconnected neural masses, where each neural mass represents a large population of excitatory and inhibitory neurons generating an EEG (or MEG) like signal. The model was recently employed in two other graph theoretical studies [18,69]. The basic unit of the model is a neural mass (NM) of the alpha rhythm [7072]. This model considers the average activity in relatively large groups of interacting excitatory and inhibitory neurons. Spatial effects (i.e. distance) are ignored in this model; brain topology is introduced by coupling multiple NMs together. The average membrane potential and spike density of the excitatory neurons of each of the NMs separately were the multichannel output that was subject to further analysis.

Network structure: Human network topology

A diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) based study by Gong et al. published in 2009 that focused on large-scale structural connectivity of the human cortex resulted in a connectivity matrix of 78 cortical regions [73,74]. The connectivity matrix was implemented in our model software, and used as topological framework for the 78 coupled NMs. Coupling between two NMs, if present, was always reciprocal, and excitatory. Note that at the start of the simulation, the coupling strength between all NM pairs was identical, and the only difference between the cortical regions (or NMs) was their degree of connectivity to other NMs (cortical regions). Please refer to the supporting information for full details.

Network degeneration: Activity-dependent degeneration (ADD)

The neural mass model described above was extended to be able to deal with activity dependent evolution of connection strength between multiple coupled NMs. Activity dependent degeneration (ADD) was realized by lowering the ‘synaptic’ coupling strength as a function of the spike density of the main excitatory neurons (all neural mass model parameters and functions are summarized and explained in S1 Text), in a similar way as previously described (18). The effects of ADD were measured by changes in ‘total power’ (local average membrane potential) and spike density, and these two measures were used as representations of neuronal activity in further analyses. The computational model is incorporated in our custom developed analysis software (‘BrainWave’, v0.9.151.5), written by C.J. Stam (available for download at http://home.kpn.nl/stam7883/brainwave.html).

Network defense: Adjustment of neuronal excitability thresholds

For the present study, we introduced counterstrategies against ADD that involve altering the neuronal excitability, either at a global level or of excitatory or inhibitory neural masses selectively. In the neural mass model, a transfer function determines the translation of membrane potential to spike density and vice versa (see S1 Text). Vm is the average membrane potential, and Vd is the threshold potential (Vd1 for excitatory, Vd2 for inhibitory neurons). Altering the level of Vd results in a sigmoid function describing the resulting spike density (see S1C Fig); a lower threshold leads to a higher spike density and vice versa. This way, the excitability of a neural mass can be changed either for excitatory or inhibitory groups selectively, or for both simultaneously.

In this model, extremely low or high neuronal excitability levels (Vd1 or Vd2 lower than 4 or higher than 10,) cause the system to quickly reach non-functional states, either shutting down functional connectivity or generating cascades of uncontrolled activity, respectively (reminiscent of epileptic seizure activity). Therefore, these were excluded from further analysis. Within the biologically plausible range, we tested various excitability levels and their effect on network dynamics (see also Fig 2). For clarity purposes, we limited the number of strategies to six distinct types: global (both excitatory and inhibitory) stimulation (Vd1 = 6, Vd2 = 6), global inhibition (Vd1 = 8, Vd2 = 8), selective stimulation of excitatory neurons (Vd1 = 5, Vd2 = 7), selective stimulation of inhibitory neurons (Vd1 = 7, Vd2 = 5), selective inhibition of excitatory neurons (Vd1 = 8, Vd2 = 7), and selective inhibition of inhibitory neurons (Vd1 = 7, Vd2 = 6.5). The strategies consisted of maintaining constant neuronal excitability levels; the initial settings of a strategy did not change over time during the degeneration period. Since for each of these interventions the threshold potential adjustment is arbitrary, we conducted simulations with six different values within each category (for example, for global stimulation six Vd1 settings between 4 and 6.5 were used, with a 0.5 increment), and compared the findings to pick a representative value. See S2 Text for an illustration of this analysis. All intervention strategies were initiated after ten degeneration cycles without any therapy, at T = 11, to simulate the manifestation of disease and subsequent therapy initiation with some delay. Model output is generated for >50 cycles of the degeneration algorithm, to simulate a progressive neurodegenerative process over time. Therefore, the X-axis variable has been defined as ‘virtual time’. Note that there is no defined relation to real time; this parameter should not be interpreted as hours, days or otherwise. In this study, we compared relative differences within the model.

Outcome measures

Spectral analysis.

As spectral analysis is a common quantitative neurophysiological procedure that provides clinically relevant information in neurodegenerative dementia, we included this in our experiments. Fast Fourier transformation of the EEG-like oscillatory output signal was used to calculate relative power. Since this neural mass model mainly generates oscillatory activity in the alpha range, the relative power analysis results should not be directly compared to patient data, but mainly serve to illustrate the changes over time in the model. Relative lower alpha power and peak frequency were included because of the well-known gradual decreases observed in AD patients[1].

Functional connectivity analysis.

The Phase Lag Index (PLI) is a measure of the asymmetry of the distribution of phase differences between two signals. It reflects the consistency with which one signal is phase leading or lagging with respect to another signal [41]. The PLI performs at least as well as the synchronization likelihood (SL) in detecting true changes in synchronization but it is much less affected by the influence of common sources [75].

The PLI is defined as index of the asymmetry of the phase difference distribution by means of in which t refers to time. If the phase difference Δϕ between two signals is in the interval 0 < Δϕ < π, the sinus function will produce a positive, non-zero value. The PLI is bounded 0 ≤ PLI ≤ 1 and a PLI of zero indicates either no coupling or coupling with a phase difference centred around 0 mod π, which may be caused by volume conduction. On the other hand, the stronger the non-zero phase locking is, the larger the PLI will be; a PLI of 1 indicates perfect phase locking, i.e. functional connectivity. For this study, PLI is calculated for all pairwise nodes and then averaged, resulting in one global value per analysis.

Graph theoretical analysis.

Network topology measures were included in our analysis based on previous graph theoretical literature in AD, and on previous experience. Although measure definition and network comparison is a methodological challenge, our choice of measures aims to give an overall impression of changing network topology. To characterize relevant changes in functional network organization we examined four aspects: the connectivity strength between neighboring nodes (normalized weighted clustering coefficient or ‘gamma’ [49]), the existence of subnetworks (modularity [66,76], the robustness against damage (algebraic connectivity [52,53]), and the existence of hub structure in the network (minimum spanning tree leaf number analysis [77]). To investigate the effect on structural network topology, we looked at the decrease in structural connectivity in nodes compared to their original connectivity strength (normalized node strength [18]). All used measures are summarized in Table 1.

Statistical analysis.

The various intervention strategies were compared with a ‘no intervention’ condition, in which the ADD algorithm was used without countering intervention, and with a ‘control’ condition, in which the simulation was run without ADD algorithm or intervention. In all conditions the structural and dynamic network at baseline was completely identical, and the only changes made were the inclusion of the ADD degeneration algorithm and the adjustment of the membrane threshold potential (Vd) value corresponding to the specific strategy.

To enable statistical comparison between the various intervention strategies with the ‘healthy control’ situation and with the ADD simulation without intervention, all different conditions were simulated ten times. These results were averaged to obtain a single representative value for each measure (for each time point). For the measures that produce results per node, the overall mean value of the whole network was used for further analysis. Standard two-tailed t-tests were used to obtain significant differences between strategies and the control conditions. A measure was considered within a ‘normal’ range when it was not significantly lower than the control condition at a certain point in time.

For evaluation and comparison of the strategies at a glance, we have summarized their results per category (neuronal activity, functional connectivity, network topology) in a global ‘performance score’ (see Fig 7). This score is derived by summing the amount of virtual time that markers within that category stay above or near a normal level (i.e. are not significantly lower than the ‘control’ condition), which is then divided by the ‘no intervention’ score of that category to derive a performance ratio.

Supporting information

S1 Fig.

(A) Schematic presentation of single neural mass model. The upper rectangle represents a mass of excitatory neurons, the lower rectangle a mass of inhibitory neurons. The state of each mass is modeled by an average membrane potential [Ve(t) and Vi(t)] and a pulse density [E(t) and I(t)]. Membrane potentials are converted to pulse densities by sigmoid functions S1[x] and S2[x]. Pulse densities are converted to membrane potentials by impulse responses he(t) and hi(t). C1 and C2 are coupling strengths between the two populations. P(t) and Ej(t) are pulse densities coming from thalamic sources or other cortical areas respectively. (B) Coupling of two neural masses. Two masses are coupled via excitatory connections. These are characterized by a fixed delay T and a strength g. (C) Essential functions of the model. The upper left panel shows the excitatory [he(t)] and inhibitory [hi(t)] impulse responses of Eq. 1. The upper right shows the sigmoid function relating average membrane potential to spike density (Eq. 2). (D) Overview of neural mass model parameters.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005707.s001

(DOC)

S2 Fig.

(A) The effect of Vd level on lower alpha relative power during the ‘stimulation of excitatory neurons’ scenario. (B) The effect of Vd level on peak frequency during the Stimulation of excitatory neurons scenario. (C) The effect of varying Vd levels on PLI during the 'Stimulation of excitatory neurons' strategy. A lower Vd1 seems related to a longer lasting normal PLI level. (D) The effect of Vd level on the normalized clustering coefficient (gamma, a measure of local connectivity) during the 'Stimulation of exctiatory neurons' scenario. (E) The effect of Vd level on algebraic connectivity (robustness) during the 'Stimulation of excitatory neurons' scenario. (F) The effect of Vd level on modularity (subnetwork presence) during the 'Stimulation of excitatory neurons' scenario. (G) The effect of Vd level on MST Leaf Number (network hub presence) during the 'Stimulation of excitatory neurons' scenario.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005707.s002

(DOC)

S3 Fig. Comparison of total power values (as a measure of neuronal activity) over time during the "Stimulation of excitatory neurons' scenario (blue) and the 'No intervention' state (red).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005707.s003

(TIF)

S2 Text. Membrane threshold potential (Vd) selection.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005707.s005

(DOC)

S3 Text. Single neural mass activity comparison between the ‘Stimulation of excitatory neurons’ scenario and the ‘No intervention’ state.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005707.s006

(DOC)

References

  1. 1. Jeong J. EEG dynamics in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Neurophysiol. Netherlands; 2004;115: 1490–1505. pmid:15203050
  2. 2. Dauwels J, Vialatte F, Cichocki A. Diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease from EEG signals: where are we standing? Curr Alzheimer Res. 2010;7: 487–505. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20455865 pmid:20455865
  3. 3. Stam CJ. Use of magnetoencephalography (MEG) to study functional brain networks in neurodegenerative disorders. J Neurol Sci. 2010;289: 128–134. pmid:19729174
  4. 4. Busche MA, Konnerth A. Neuronal hyperactivity—A key defect in Alzheimer’s disease? BioEssays. 2015; n/a-n/a. pmid:25773221
  5. 5. Pijnenburg YAL, v d Made Y, van Cappellen van Walsum AM, Knol DL, Scheltens P, Stam CJ. EEG synchronization likelihood in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease during a working memory task. Clin Neurophysiol. Netherlands; 2004;115: 1332–1339. pmid:15134700
  6. 6. Maestu F, Baykova E, Ruiz JMM, Montejo P, Montenegro M, Llanero M, et al. Increased biomagnetic activity in healthy elderly with subjective memory complaints. Clin Neurophysiol. International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology; 2011;122: 499–505. pmid:20826109
  7. 7. Maestú F, Peña J-M, Garcés P, González S, Bajo R, Bagic A, et al. A multicenter study of the early detection of synaptic dysfunction in Mild Cognitive Impairment using Magnetoencephalography-derived functional connectivity. NeuroImage Clin. 2015;9: 103–9. pmid:26448910
  8. 8. Dickerson BC, Salat DH, Greve DN, Chua EF, Rand-Giovannetti E, Rentz DM, et al. Increased hippocampal activation in mild cognitive impairment compared to normal aging and AD. Neurology. AAN Enterprises; 2005;65: 404. pmid:16087905
  9. 9. Sperling RA, Dickerson BC, Pihlajamaki M, Vannini P, LaViolette PS, Vitolo O V, et al. Functional alterations in memory networks in early Alzheimer’s disease. Neuromolecular Med. United States; 2010;12: 27–43. pmid:20069392
  10. 10. Celone K a, Calhoun VD, Dickerson BC, Atri A, Chua EF, Miller SL, et al. Alterations in memory networks in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: an independent component analysis. J Neurosci. United States; 2006;26: 10222–10231. pmid:17021177
  11. 11. Jones DT, Knopman DS, Gunter JL, Graff-Radford J, Vemuri P, Boeve BF, et al. Cascading network failure across the Alzheimer’s disease spectrum. Brain. 2015; awv338-. pmid:26586695
  12. 12. Palop JJ, Mucke L. Amyloid-[beta]-induced neuronal dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: from synapses toward neural networks. Nat Neurosci. Nature Publishing Group; 2010;13: 812–818. pmid:20581818
  13. 13. Gleichmann M, Mattson MP. Alzheimers Disease and Neuronal Network Activity. NeuroMolecular Med. Humana Press Inc.; 2010;12: 44–47. pmid:19885650
  14. 14. Bero AW, Yan P, Roh JH, Cirrito JR, Stewart FR, Raichle ME, et al. Neuronal activity regulates the regional vulnerability to amyloid-β deposition. Nat Neurosci. United States; 2011;14: 750–756. pmid:21532579
  15. 15. Cirrito JR, Yamada KA, Finn MB, Sloviter RS, Bales KR, May PC, et al. Synaptic activity regulates interstitial fluid amyloid-[beta] levels in vivo. Neuron. Elsevier; 2005;48: 913–922. pmid:16364896
  16. 16. Nimmrich V, Draguhn A, Axmacher N. Neuronal Network Oscillations in Neurodegenerative Diseases. Neuromolecular Med. 2015; pmid:25920466
  17. 17. Gleichmann M, Chow VW, Mattson MP. Homeostatic Disinhibition in the Aging Brain and Alzheimer’s Disease. J Alzheimer’s Dis. IOS Press; 2011;24: 15–24.
  18. 18. de Haan W, Mott K, van Straaten ECW, Scheltens P, Stam CJ. Activity Dependent Degeneration Explains Hub Vulnerability in Alzheimer’s Disease. PLoS Comput Biol. 2012;8.
  19. 19. Pihlajamäki M, Sperling RA. Functional MRI assessment of task-induced deactivation of the default mode network in Alzheimer’s disease and at-risk older individuals. Behav Neurol. Netherlands; 2009;21: 77–91. pmid:19847047
  20. 20. Sheline YI, Morris JC, Snyder AZ, Price JL, Yan Z, D’Angelo G, et al. APOE4 allele disrupts resting state fMRI connectivity in the absence of amyloid plaques or decreased CSF Aβ42. J Neurosci. United States; 2010;30: 17035–17040. pmid:21159973
  21. 21. Prichep LS, John ER, Ferris SH, Rausch L, Fang Z, Cancro R, et al. Prediction of longitudinal cognitive decline in normal elderly with subjective complaints using electrophysiological imaging. Neurobiol Aging. United States; 2006;27: 471–481. pmid:16213630
  22. 22. Buldú JM, Bajo R, Maestú F, Castellanos N, Leyva I, Gil P, et al. Reorganization of functional networks in mild cognitive impairment. PLoS One. United States; 2011;6: e19584. pmid:21625430
  23. 23. Criado JR, Amo C, Quint P, Kurelowech L, Otis SM. Using magnetoencephalography to study patterns of brain magnetic activity in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. United States; 2006;21: 416–423. pmid:17267374
  24. 24. Filippini N, MacIntosh BJ, Hough MG, Goodwin GM, Frisoni GB, Smith SM, et al. Distinct patterns of brain activity in young carriers of the APOE-varepsilon4 allele. Proc Natl Acad Sci. National Acad Sciences; 2009;106: 7209. pmid:19357304
  25. 25. Machulda MM, Jones DT, Vemuri P, McDade E, Avula R, Przybelski S, et al. Effect of APOE {varepsilon}4 Status on Intrinsic Network Connectivity in Cognitively Normal Elderly Subjects. Arch Neurol. 2011;68: 1131–1136. pmid:21555604
  26. 26. Palop JJ, Mucke L. Epilepsy and cognitive impairments in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. NIH Public Access; 2009;66: 435. pmid:19204149
  27. 27. Larner AJ. Epileptic seizures in AD patients. Neuromolecular Med. United States; 2010;12: 71–77. pmid:19557550
  28. 28. Olin J, Schneider L. Galantamine for Alzheimer’s disease. Cochrane database Syst Rev. 2002; CD001747. pmid:12137632
  29. 29. Birks JS, Grimley Evans J. Rivastigmine for Alzheimer’s disease. Cochrane database Syst Rev. 2015;4: CD001191. pmid:25858345
  30. 30. Areosa SA, Sherriff F. Memantine for dementia. Cochrane database Syst Rev. 2003; CD003154.
  31. 31. Bakker A, Krauss GL, Albert MS, Speck CL, Jones LR, Stark CE, et al. Reduction of Hippocampal Hyperactivity Improves Cognition in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. Neuron. 2012;74: 467–474. pmid:22578498
  32. 32. Scheltens P, Twisk JWR, Blesa R, Scarpini E, von Arnim CAF, Bongers A, et al. Efficacy of Souvenaid in mild Alzheimer’s disease: results from a randomized, controlled trial. J Alzheimers Dis. IOS Press; 2012;31: 225–36. pmid:22766770
  33. 33. de Waal H, Stam CJ, Lansbergen MM, Wieggers RL, Kamphuis PJGH, Scheltens P, et al. The effect of souvenaid on functional brain network organisation in patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease: a randomised controlled study. PLoS One. 2014;9: e86558. pmid:24475144
  34. 34. Laxton AW, Tang-Wai DF, McAndrews MP, Zumsteg D, Wennberg R, Keren R, et al. A phase I trial of deep brain stimulation of memory circuits in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol. United States; 2010;68: 521–534. pmid:20687206
  35. 35. Smith GS, Laxton AW, Tang-Wai DF, McAndrews MP, Diaconescu AO, Workman CI, et al. Increased Cerebral Metabolism After 1 Year of Deep Brain Stimulation in Alzheimer Disease. Arch Neurol. 2012; pmid:22566505
  36. 36. Nardone R, Bergmann J, Christova M, Caleri F, Tezzon F, Ladurner G, et al. Effect of transcranial brain stimulation for the treatment of Alzheimer disease: a review. Int J Alzheimers Dis. 2012;2012: 687909. pmid:22114748
  37. 37. Izhikevich EM, Edelman GM. Large-scale model of mammalian thalamocortical systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2008;105: 3593–3598. pmid:18292226
  38. 38. Alstott J, Breakspear M, Hagmann P, Cammoun L, Sporns O. Modeling the impact of lesions in the human brain. PLoS Comput Biol. United States; 2009;5: e1000408. pmid:19521503
  39. 39. Bullmore E, Sporns O. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci. England; 2009;10: 186–198. pmid:19190637
  40. 40. Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage. United States; 2010;52: 1059–1069. pmid:19819337
  41. 41. Stam CJ, Nolte G, Daffertshofer A. Phase lag index: assessment of functional connectivity from multi channel EEG and MEG with diminished bias from common sources. Hum Brain Mapp. United States; 2007;28: 1178–1193. pmid:17266107
  42. 42. Bassett DS, Bullmore ET. Human brain networks in health and disease. Curr Opin Neurol. England; 2009;22: 340–347. pmid:19494774
  43. 43. Fornito A, Zalesky A, Breakspear M. The connectomics of brain disorders. Nat Publ Gr. Nature Publishing Group; 2015;16: 159–172. pmid:25697159
  44. 44. Arsiwalla XD, Zucca R, Betella A, Martinez E, Dalmazzo D, Omedas P, et al. Network dynamics with BrainX3: a large-scale simulation of the human brain network with real-time interaction. Front Neuroinform. 2015;9: 2. pmid:25759649
  45. 45. Boes AD, Prasad S, Liu H, Liu Q, Pascual-Leone A, Caviness VS, et al. Network localization of neurological symptoms from focal brain lesions. Brain. 2015; awv228. pmid:26264514
  46. 46. Honey CJ, Sporns O. Dynamical consequences of lesions in cortical networks. Hum Brain Mapp. United States; 2008;29: 802–809. pmid:18438885
  47. 47. Crossley NA, Mechelli A, Scott J, Carletti F, Fox PT, Mcguire P, et al. The hubs of the human connectome are generally implicated in the anatomy of brain disorders. Brain. 2014;137: 2382–2395. pmid:25057133
  48. 48. Stam CJ. No Title. Niedermeyer’s Electroencephalography Basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields. 2011. pp. 375–393.
  49. 49. Stam CJ, de Haan W, Daffertshofer A, Jones BF, Manshanden I, van Cappellen van Walsum a M, et al. Graph theoretical analysis of magnetoencephalographic functional connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2009;132: 213–24. pmid:18952674
  50. 50. de Haan W, Pijnenburg YAL, Strijers RLM, van der Made Y, van der Flier WM, Scheltens P, et al. Functional neural network analysis in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease using EEG and graph theory. BMC Neurosci. England; 2009;10: 101. pmid:19698093
  51. 51. de Haan W, van der Flier WM, Koene T, Smits LL, Scheltens P, Stam CJ. Disrupted modular brain dynamics reflect cognitive dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimage. United States; 2011;59: 3085–3093. pmid:22154957
  52. 52. de Haan W, van der Flier WM, Wang H, Van Mieghem PF a., Scheltens P, Stam CJ. Disruption of Functional Brain Networks in Alzheimer’s Disease: What Can We Learn from Graph Spectral Analysis of Resting-State Magnetoencephalography? Brain Connect. 2012;2: 45–55. pmid:22480296
  53. 53. Fiedler M. Algebraic connectivity of graphs. Czechoslov Math J. Institute of Mathematics, Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic; 1973;23: 298–305.
  54. 54. Tijms BM, Wink AM, de Haan W, van der Flier WM, Stam CJ, Scheltens P, et al. Alzheimer’s disease: connecting findings from graph theoretical studies of brain networks. Neurobiology of Aging. 2013. pp. 2023–2036. pmid:23541878
  55. 55. Sanchez PE, Zhu L, Verret L, Vossel KA, Orr AG, Cirrito JR, et al. Levetiracetam suppresses neuronal network dysfunction and reverses synaptic and cognitive deficits in an Alzheimer’s disease model. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109: E2895–903. pmid:22869752
  56. 56. Stam CJ. Modern network science of neurological disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci. Nature Publishing Group, a division of Macmillan Publishers Limited. All Rights Reserved.; 2014;15: 683–695. pmid:25186238
  57. 57. Palop JJ, Mucke L. Network abnormalities and interneuron dysfunction in Alzheimer disease. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2016;17: 777–792. pmid:27829687
  58. 58. Canter RG, Penney J, Tsai L-H. The road to restoring neural circuits for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Nature. Nature Research; 2016;539: 187–196. pmid:27830780
  59. 59. Haberman RP, Branch A, Gallagher M. Targeting Neural Hyperactivity as a Treatment to Stem Progression of Late-Onset Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurotherapeutics. Springer US; 2017; 1–15.
  60. 60. Dubois B, Hampel H, Feldman HH, Scheltens P, Aisen P, Andrieu S, et al. Preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: Definition, natural history, and diagnostic criteria. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2016;12: 292–323. pmid:27012484
  61. 61. Wedeen VJ, Wang RP, Schmahmann JD, Benner T, Tseng WYI, Dai G, et al. Diffusion spectrum magnetic resonance imaging (DSI) tractography of crossing fibers. Neuroimage. 2008;41: 1267–1277. pmid:18495497
  62. 62. Wang JY, Abdi H, Bakhadirov K, Diaz-Arrastia R, Devous MD. A comprehensive reliability assessment of quantitative diffusion tensor tractography. Neuroimage. 2012;60: 1127–1138. pmid:22227883
  63. 63. Henriksen S, Pang R, Wronkiewicz M. A simple generative model of the mouse mesoscale connectome. Elife. 2016;5. pmid:26978793
  64. 64. Vertes PE, Alexander-Bloch a. F, Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Rapoport JL, Bullmore ET. Simple models of human brain functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2012;109: 5868–5873. pmid:22467830
  65. 65. Betzel RF, Avena-Koenigsberger A, Goñi J, He Y, de Reus MA, Griffa A, et al. Generative models of the human connectome. Neuroimage. 2016;124: 1054–1064. pmid:26427642
  66. 66. Stam CJ, Hillebrand A, Wang H, Van Mieghem P. Emergence of Modular Structure in a Large-Scale Brain Network with Interactions between Dynamics and Connectivity. Front Comput Neurosci. Switzerland; 2010;4. pmid:20953245
  67. 67. Gomez-Ramirez J, Wu J. Network-Based Biomarkers in Alzheimer’s Disease: Review and Future Directions. Front Aging Neurosci. Frontiers; 2014;6. pmid:24550828
  68. 68. Teng Xie YH. Mapping the Alzheimer?’s Brain with Connectomics. Front Psychiatry. Frontiers Media SA; 2011;2.
  69. 69. Ponten SC, Daffertshofer a., Hillebrand a., Stam CJ. The relationship between structural and functional connectivity: graph theoretical analysis of an EEG neural mass model. Neuroimage. United States: Elsevier B.V.; 2010;52: 985–994. pmid:19853665
  70. 70. da Silva FH, Hoeks A, Smits H, Zetterberg LH. Model of brain rhythmic activity. Biol Cybern. Springer; 1974;15: 27–37.
  71. 71. Zetterberg LH, Kristiansson L, Mossberg K. Performance of a model for a local neuron population. Biol Cybern. Springer; 1978;31: 15–26. pmid:728488
  72. 72. Stam CJ, Pijn JP, Suffczynski P, da Silva FH. Dynamics of the human alpha rhythm: evidence for non-linearity? Clin Neurophysiol. NETHERLANDS; 1999;110: 1801–1813. pmid:10574295
  73. 73. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. United States; 2002;15: 273–289. pmid:11771995
  74. 74. Gong G, He Y, Concha L, Lebel C, Gross DW, Evans AC, et al. Mapping anatomical connectivity patterns of human cerebral cortex using in vivo diffusion tensor imaging tractography. Cereb Cortex. United States; 2009;19: 524–536. pmid:18567609
  75. 75. Montez T, Linkenkaer-Hansen K, van Dijk BW, Stam CJ. Synchronization likelihood with explicit time-frequency priors. Neuroimage. United States; 2006;33: 1117–1125. pmid:17023181
  76. 76. Amaral L a, Scala a, Barthelemy M, Stanley HE. Classes of small-world networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97: 11149–52. pmid:11005838
  77. 77. Stam CJ, Tewarie P, Van Dellen E, van Straaten ECW, Hillebrand A, Van Mieghem P. The trees and the forest: Characterization of complex brain networks with minimum spanning trees. Int J Psychophysiol. 2014;92: 129–38. pmid:24726900